Belyaev’s Fox Experiment – Answers – Part III

This post is part of the series on Belyaev’s fox experiments.
(index | part I | part II | part III | part IV )

There are several theories that have been put forward regarding the origins of the physical characteristics seen in Belyaev’s foxes.  I will rebut some theories, and consider the plausibility of others. There are no definite answers, just some realistic theories.

Experimenter bias

One of the most popular anecdotal suggestions is that perhaps the experimenters unconsciously selected for more dog-like physical characteristics.  Personally, I think this illustrates a lack-of-faith in the scientific method.  The nature of the tests has been clearly outlined, and we must have confidence that this method was adhered to.  If there were serious doubts, the method allows for replication.  As replication has not occurred (mostly due to expense inhibiting the experiment) we can conclude that the experiment’s results are plausible in its current form. (The empirical world loves nothing more than proving others ‘wrong’ through replication.)

Mutations and Inbreeding

Another loud argument is the notion that the initial stock was carrying mutations or unusual traits, or that these changes were as a result of mutations.  Because of the inbreeding of the experiment, these mutations were amplified. This can be rebutted in a number of ways.

Firstly, the foxes were not inbred.  This fox population was frequently outcrossed to other commercial fox farm stock, and this has meant that the domesticated fox population has an inbreeding coefficient of 0.02 to 0.07.

Secondly, many of the novel traits outlined in part II are in fact not recessive. This means that the foundation population’s mutations would have been apparent on commencement of the experiment. This was not the case – these traits became apparent over the course of the experiment, and not in the beginning stages.

Another idea is that random mutations are the cause of these traits.  However, Belyaev determined that the rate of change in the domesticated strain was “2 or 3 orders higher than the expected frequency of spontaneous mutations”. This means that it probably not mutations that have caused the changes documented.

If we consider the mutation route as plausible, the suggestion with the most worth is Vavilov’s theory of homologous variability.  Vavilov’s theory suggested that similar gene sets can give rise to similar mutations, and so we can apply the term ‘similar gene set’ to all foxes, and mutations to their unique traits. This explains how foxes, despite being unrelated, developed similar traits just by the nature of being a fox with a fox gene set.

However, mutations probably did not have a role in the changes seem in the foxes.  What is more likely is that behaviour and anatomy may be linked in some way.

Depigmentation is a characteristic in dogs that was also seen in domesticated foxes. Photos © Ruthless Photos

Depigmentation is a characteristic in dogs that was also seen in domesticated foxes.Photos © Ruthless Photos

Selecting for many genes

The behaviour of ‘tameness’ is a varied trait, and so is controlled by a number of genes.  Because there are a number of genes involved, this means that selecting for tameness, and so also a number of genes, could have a profound affect.

Selecting for important genes

However, what is a more convincing suggestion is that perhaps this rapid change may have been as a result of selection may have been acting on relatively few genes, and genes that have an important regulatory role.  This would mean that if a ‘master’ gene was being selected for, this could have far reaching implications.  Here we reach the most convincing theory: That selecting for tameness was selecting for a major, complex, hormonal regulatory gene (or genes) which has far ranging implications on the rest of the animal.

The traits in foxes are found in many different domesticated species. Because of these similarities, Belyaev thought that early changes for amenability to domestication must be related to domesticated physiologies.  Because behaviour is regulated by neurotransmitters and hormones, modifying these elements through selecting behaviour would also have affects physiological parts of the animal.  Even though mammals are varied, their physiological processes are quite similar (their hormones, neurotransmitters, etc) – so this would be the basis for many domesticated mammal species showing similar traits.

Domestication/tamability is behaviour that is rooted in physiological changes and systems (e.g. hormones and neurochemicals).  Changing these complex systems would have far-reaching effects on the development of the animals themselves. And as all mammals are controlled by similar bigger-regulatory systems, this is seen as a reasonable explanation for the changes.

Hormone selection

‘Tameness’, ‘nervousness’ and ‘aggression’ is probably controlled by the endocrine system.  As described in the last post, serotonin, corticosteroid, cortisol, and adrenocorticotropic hormone were all found to be reduced in domesticated foxes. These hormones are responsible for behaviour that was selected for.  However, these hormones have a much bigger role in the endocrine system, so selecting for hormones would have had an extensive role and account for many of the changes observed in the domesticated animals.

Indeed, even the colour changes seen can be accounted for by hormones.  Hormones are linked to pigmentogenesis, agouti, and melanin.  The endocratic system can also explain the moulting changes in the domesticated foxes.

The endocrine system can explain many of the changes in the domesticated foxes behaviour, but this system also has a big role in development.  In selecting for genes that control behaviour, selection was also made for genes that control development.

The presence of juvenille traits (e.g play) in adult dogs was also seen in foxes. Photos © Ruthless Photos

Development mechanisms

The characteristics that the foxes adopted are those that are similar to juveniellism.  In this way, the development of the domesticated foxes can be described as ‘retarded’, as even adults have juvenile behaviours.  In this way it is thought that genes responsible for development have in some way been selected for.

These developmental changes start from embryos, with the hormones already described affecting the whole development process.  Even colouration/pigmentation has been linked to melanocyte and melanoblast activity in embryonic stage.  Neucrocrest cell migration would be delayed, which means messages to mature would not get to some body parts.  This would also have implications to the socialisation period, and be responsible for the floppy ears. Changes in the maturation timing have been seen.

Behaviour selected for seems to have been controlled by a few genes, but these genes were also responsible for a high level of regulation (i.e. hormonal level, and influencing development) and hence the foxes had a range of phenotype changes that accompanied the selected behaviours.

This is what domestication looks like

The literature review strongly stated that domesticated foxes, and their characteristics, are not terribly surprising.  Belyaev says that the “data demonstrate for foxes the kind of variability in similar characters and functions that is often observed in the domestication of other species of animals.” Because all domesticated animals have ‘done the same thing’ (in terms of phenotype traits), then this must be an implication of domestication and not an innate genetic quality of the fox population.

It is from these conclusions that the next post will start to make conclusions that relate to the domestication of dogs.  As our dogs display the domestication characteristics of the fox, this experiment is valid to our understanding of the history of dogs.

References:

 

Belyaev, DK 1979, ‘Destablizing selection as a factor in domestication’, Journal of Heredity, vol 70, iss 5, pp301-308. [link]

 

Gilbert, SF, ‘Evolution and domestication: selection on developmental genes?’, in Developmental Biology (8th edition). [link]

 

Kukekova, AV, Acland, GM, Oskina, IN, Kharlamova, AV, Trut, LN, Chase, K, Lark, KG, Hollis, NE, & Aguirre, GD 2006, ‘The genetics of domesticated behavior in canids: What can dogs and silver foxes tell us about each other?’, The Dog and Its Genome, Cold Spring Harbor Labratory Press. [link]

 

Kukekova, AV, Trut, LN, Oskina, IN, Johnson, JL, Temnykh, SV, Khariamova, AV, Shepeleva, DV, Gulievich, RG, Shikhevich, SG, Graphodatsky, AS, Aguirre, GD & Acland, GM 2007, ‘A meiotic linkage map of the silver fox, aligned and compared to the canine genome’, Genome Research, vol 17, pp387-399. [link]

“New Nice” on the Radiolab show in October 2009. [link] (I highly recommend this reference for a light hearted, audio version of this experiment.)

 

Trut, LN, Kharlamova, AV, Kukekova, AV, Acland, GM, Carrier, DR, Chase, K, & Lark, KG 2006, ‘Morphology and behavior: are they coupled at the genome level?’, The Dog and Its Genome, Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press, Wdoobury, NY: 81-93. [link]

Trut, L 1999, ‘Early canid domestication: The farm-fox experiment’, American Scientist, vol 87, no 2, p160.  [link](I highly recommend this reference for further reading.)

Trut LN. 2001. Experimental Studies of Early Canid Domestication. In The Genetics of the Dog (ed A Ruvinksy and J Sampson). CABI 2001, p.15-43.

 

11 thoughts on “Belyaev’s Fox Experiment – Answers – Part III

  1. So hey, I’m only an undergraduate right now, but I actually have a question about that inbreeding statistic that I’ve been trying to answer all night. How on earth did they come up with that CoI number? I’ve been looking through the primary literature and the papers you cited with no actual discussion of how many outcrosses they did, how the CoI was calculated (how many generations of pedigree went into the analysis for each animal?), or how frequently outcrosses to other fur farms were used.

    I ask because the papers I’ve found are also claiming some pretty strong selection rates per generation, with only 5% of males and 20% of females breeding on. If this is true, they’re losing 80% minimum of the population’s genetic diversity every single generation, minus whatever is coming in with an outcross. You’d need to do a lot of outcrosses and you’d need to do them very frequently to bring the CoI for each population down that low if you’re losing that much genetic diversity in each successive generation of your main population.

    Anyway, my main question is this: Do you know anything about how they calculated that CoI or how many generations were used to calculate it? Do you know how they organized outcrossing? I would really, really like to find this information because the level of selection they’re imposing generation after generation and that low a CoI just do not ring true to me at all, at least not without doing some pretty massive outcrossing. All I can find is essentially “we did outcrossing, and here are our CoIs!” without any discussion of where that number actually came from or discussion of methodology.

    • Hi Erin. Thanks for reading and for your question. I can only refer to to my sources, but I am not sure if they actually answer the question you’re looking for. I think you’re right – they do ‘just’ say “We did outcrossing and here are the COIs”, as you put it. 🙂

      Belyaev 1979 (as linked above) and Trut 1999 (also linked above) are where they said “outcrossing was used” and then “here are our COIs” (respectively).

      The animals from other farms were “selected for behaviour” (Belyaev 1979), but it doesn’t go into more details. I am guessing they found the tamest they could from farms to reintroduce, but it doesn’t explicitly say this.

      Maybe you can try contacting Lyudmila Trut regarding this? Her contact details are here: http://www.americanscientist.org/authors/detail/lyudmila-trut

      • Bugger, I was afraid you’d say that. Thanks for the help, though! I’ll look into sending her an email.

        • Would love to hear more details about what you’re doing. 🙂 Please get in touch if I can help, and if you find an answer! And also with any research you’re doing. What’s your project?

          • Hah, this is all personal curiosity. I was reading up on the Belyaev experiment as part of a discussion I was having with a friend, and then a minor point of the discussion got me looking for a good overview of the methods of the experiment, which were surprisingly difficult to find for such a widely discussed and widely taught experiment. And then I saw that level of selection numbers coupled with that CoI and went “….what?” It seems pretty counter-intuitive that they’re constricting the population that much and are yet maintaining it as so outbred, so I wanted to see if I could figure out how they calculated the statistic.

            I’ll let you know if I get an answer! I’ll be surprised if this turns into anything big, but you never know.

  2. Erin, just a thought: The juvenile traits of the foxes was shown to be dominant. In this way, outcrossing them to different populations should manage to retain their physical characteristics…

    • That sounds about right.

      It is conceivable that several generations of inbreeding, interspersed with outcrossing, could reset the coefficient of relatedness to low values. There would be high values during selective inbreeding, but then outcrossing would result in practically zero relatedness once more. Once the dominant traits had been established in a round of inbreeding, recovering them all after an outcross would be easy, and it would not even be necessary to resume strong inbreeding to reestablish the traits. A lot of foxes were involved, and pairings as close as second cousins (after outcrossing) give a coefficient of 0.03. It would be nice if the exact lineages were described in these papers, but this methodology is probably one of those standard-practice things that no-one elaborates on any more. Besides, what motive would there be for using false values here? The relatedness is practically irrelevant once you accept that intensive linebreeding was used.

  3. Sorry, “pairings as close as second cousins (after outcrossing) give a coefficient of 0.03” should be “relatives” instead of “pairings”. It is third cousin pairings that give a value of 0.016 (0.0078 x 2). It’s doable.

  4. Pingback: PetSci Awards « PetSci | Forums

Comments are closed.