11/12/12

The Week in Tweets – 12th November

Welcome to our only weekly (ish) segment: The Week in Tweets! This is where I summarise my weekly tweets (from my Twitter account) and choose my favourite as the Tweet of the Week.

 

Tweet of the Week

There were a lot of rescue posts this week that caught my attention and could become the ‘tweet of the week’, but Lindsay from ThatMutt won out with her post “Community support for no kill“. To me, this is a perfect example of no-kill, open-admission sheltering at work.  In an emergency situation, Animal Allies Humane Society took in 63 cats.  They made room, not by killing, but by waiving adoption fees and going to the media and community for help.  A brilliant good news story that makes me believe in the possibilities of no-kill.

Continue reading

11/10/12

Preparing for Puppies

So we have been undertaking puppy preparations here, in anticipation for Clover’s upcoming litter. We have many basic things, as we have had litters before – thermometers, heat lamp, whelping box, and so forth. So, really, the only stuff we had to get is the fun stuff!

 

Leave with Work

I put in leave with work, as I pretty much don’t work when I have puppies. I need to be here for the whelping, and I need to be here to clean, socialise, and just generally care take puppies. At a young age, they are not too strenuous. As they get older, the elimination increases, and so does the work load!

 

Clover in the whelping box, the blanket over the top is to make it more den like for her. The whelping box is next to our bed. You can see to the right the towels ‘ready to go’ on top of a column heater. The brown blanket is lining a box, which has a hot water bottle inside – ready for puppies when they’re born. The blue toy outside the whelping box is Clover’s personal touch!

Continue reading

11/4/12

Schedules of Reinforcement

This post is part of the series in response to Dunbar’s 2012 Australian seminars. See index.

 

When training dogs, consciously or not, we undertake a pattern of delivering rewards.  Sometimes, we give a dog a reward for every correct behaviour, sometimes the dog gets the reward for 3 times they get it right, or sometimes we mix it up and we may reward a sit-stay for 3 seconds, and then 8 seconds, then 5 seconds, and so forth.

Believe it or not, at some point, all the different patterns of rewards (“schedules of reinforcement”) have been given names and classified. First, I’ll describe these schedules of reinforcement, and then the ‘better’ alternative (according to Dunbar but also according to me!).

 

Continuous Reinforcement

Continuous reinforcement means rewarding a dog every time a response is performed.

Example:  Every time you call your dog, you reward them with a treat.

Pros: This often builds up a very high level of response, as the dog understands they will get a treat, and so are highly motivated to accomplish the behaviour.

Cons: It’s easy to run out of treats!  Because of the nature of the schedule, you reward everything – including ‘sloppy’ or ‘slow responses’.

 

Fixed Schedule – Ratio or Interval

A fixed schedule means that the reward is delivered on a consistence basis, though intermittently.  Ratio or interval refers to number of responses and time passed during response respectively.

Example: Fixed Ratio: When your dog barks at the door, you normally ignore it the first and second time, but when they bark thrice, you let them in. Interval Ratio: This is difficult to conceptualise in dog training, as it assumes that the dog is maintaining a behaviour.  The best example is training the stay: For every 10 seconds that the dog maintains a stay, you return and give the dog a treat.

Pros: Quick way for dog to learn. Good way to maintain behaviours. Prepares a dog to work without always getting rewarded (which is particularly useful for sports like obedience, where the dog cannot get rewarded in the ring).

Cons: Not as quick for dogs to understand as with continuous reinforcement.  The dog’s behaviour make become ‘scalloped’, with more enthused and motivated behaviours nearer to the reward (e.g. if the dog catches on that they only get rewarded for every 3rd sit, they will be more enthused about doing the 3rd one than the 1st and 2nd).  When the intervals or ratios are too long, the dog may ‘strike’ (i.e. quit perfoming the behaviour).  Ratios are difficult to implement in practice, as it normally hard to count and train at the same time!  Moving away from a fixed schedule often demotivates the dog (e.g. if the dog is used to being rewarded at every 3rd behaviour, for this to cease, often means the dog will abruptly stop performing the behaviour).

 

Variable schedules – ratio or interval

Variable schedules are when the animal is rewarded at changing consistency.  Ratio refers to the number of repetitions inbetween rewards, and interval refers to the time period inbetween rewards.

Example:  Ratio: When you reward your dog for ‘sit’, you don’t reward them everytime. Instead, you reward them for their first successful behaviour, then you miss a couple, and reward the 4th one, then you reward the 6th behaviour, then you reward the 9th behaviour, and so forth.  (This is the principle that poker machines work on.)  Interval: Again, if you have a dog in a stay, you could reward the dog for 5 seconds having passed, then 10 seconds, then 8 seconds, then 15 seconds, then 7 seconds, and so forth. So you are rewarding the dog based on a variable time interval schedule.

Pros: This reward schedule works well and is good for maintaining behaviours.

Cons: Too complex for any person to apply.  It is very much a laboratory/computer type of training, and is too complex for people to implement.  If you stretch the ratio too far, the dog will ‘strike’.

Continue reading

10/28/12

Tweets of the Week (28th October)

This is Some Thoughts About Dogs’ weekly segment where we share all the links shared on my Twitter for the past week. Sit down with a cuppa and enjoy.

 

Tweet of the Week

Could dogs and puppies be languishing in shelters because of rescue scrutiny?  Though we all know that rescues need to screen buyers and have the absolute right, and obligation, to find the most suitable home for their animals, a long running concern of mine is that rescue groups could be too fussy.  The relatively new blog, Team Unruly, shared an article titled Not Good Enough, looking at this very issue. This blog is written by a number of talented doggy people, but Michelle is the author of this post. She talks about the rigid requirements that rescue hold, that may prevent pets from finding homes.

Continue reading

10/25/12

Tempting Fussy Eaters

First, I must say, I do not believe in catering to fussy eaters. To me, fussy eaters are made, not born, and it’s simply a bad habit that many people encourage.  For dogs that are just naughty in not eating, then I strongly recommend Sue Ailsby’s guide to teaching a dog to eat.

However, sometimes, there are medical reasons that may mean that a dog is disinclined to eat.  For Clover, her pregnancy has made her nauseous and cease to eat.  Sometimes, in times of extreme stress (such as in boarding kennels), dogs choose not to eat.  In these cases, it’s often cyclic – I know that when I don’t eat, I feel sick, and so I don’t want to eat.  I am sure the same principle applies to dogs.  There is also a rumour that dogs reduce stress by eating, so another positive in encouraging a dog to eat.

For times of medical need, or stress, I’ve compiled a list of tactics to encourage a dog to eat. These should only be used when medical illness has been ruled out and only on a short term basis.

Competition and hand feeding – two possible options for a fussy eater.

Continue reading